Why do policymakers tend to think failure is due to factors beyond their control, while citizens think policy failure is due to incompetence? A knowledge of psychology and confirmatory bias sheds light on such disagreements.
By David Chan, Published The Straits Times, 16 Nov 2013
MOST people can recall times when individuals maintain the same position despite being given new information that seems to contradict it. Sometimes, people may even claim that the contrary information presented supports their position and so strengthens their belief.
The more important the topic, the more it affects interpersonal relationships negatively when there is strong disagreement.
The more important the topic, the more it affects interpersonal relationships negatively when there is strong disagreement.
It gets worse when the issues seem clear and the individuals concerned are intelligent. In such cases, a lack of understanding cannot be the problem.
This resistance to change is part of human psychology. It applies to everyone regardless of educational background, socio-economic status, political belief and moral position.
But if the underlying psychology is understood, this knowledge can facilitate personal and workplace relationships. It can also help address disagreements between policymakers and citizens or advocacy groups.
Confirmatory bias
Confirmatory bias
EVERYONE tends to seek out, interpret and remember information that confirms existing beliefs, positions or actions. Psychologists call it confirmatory bias, a term coined by Peter Wason. In the 1970s, Professor Wason published a series of seminal studies which showed that when asked to test a simple rule, people consistently sought information that would confirm the rule and ignored information that would disprove it.
In 1979, a research team from Stanford University published a study involving participants who were either for or against capital punishment. All participants were asked to read and evaluate two studies, one supporting capital punishment and the other undermining it. The results showed that participants rated the study that was consistent with their pre-existing position as superior to the one that contradicted it. In reality, however, both studies employed the same methodology and were fictitious.
In 1979, a research team from Stanford University published a study involving participants who were either for or against capital punishment. All participants were asked to read and evaluate two studies, one supporting capital punishment and the other undermining it. The results showed that participants rated the study that was consistent with their pre-existing position as superior to the one that contradicted it. In reality, however, both studies employed the same methodology and were fictitious.
Subsequent research has shown that people express more interest and spend more time on information that is consistent with their pre-existing positions or actions. They also give more weight to and recall better such consistent information.
Confirmation bias also occurs in real life. People go about their respective routines paying more attention to news outlets and commentary articles, in mainstream or social media, which reflect their own political views. They also spend more time discussing and analysing issues with those who share their views than with those who don’t.
Does confirmatory bias occur when there is strong disagreement over an issue between policy makers and citizens or advocacy groups?
There are several clues to look for.
Look at the type of information that each side seeks out as relevant evidence for the debate. Look at which aspects of the issue they attend to. Look at how they differ when interpreting and making sense of what the same data means. And look at what each side recalls when citing previous cases they consider similar or relevant to the current issue.
Policymakers tend to believe that policy successes are largely due to their acumen, and that policy failures are largely due to changes in external conditions beyond their control.
Citizens tend to believe that policy successes are largely due to luck, public cooperation or resources available to policymakers. They see policy failures as being largely due to incompetence or an inability to plan ahead.
An example is the way in which strains on Singapore’s infrastructure and overcrowding on public transport in recent years yield different reactions.
Citizens tend to attribute such overcrowding to the lack of planning on the part of policymakers and their overemphasis on population growth. Policymakers, however, attribute the problem to unexpected changes in economic conditions. They also cite the growth in the number of foreigners, and the many years needed for building infrastructure projects.
Reacting constructively
IS THERE a way to react constructively to information that is contrary to firmly held opinions?
There are no magic bullets, but here are some possible approaches to prevent negative effects and promote positive ones.
- In all major decisions, make a serious effort to question or at least revisit one’s assumptions.
- Seek information that undermines and not just confirms pre-existing beliefs and positions.
- In group discussions, do not express a position before hearing from the other members. This allows alternative, and possibly better, ideas to surface more easily. This is especially important when leading a group, or if group members have a similar profile.
- Spend less time getting the views and listening to the justifications provided by like-minded individuals.
- Create more opportunities to listen to the views of those who may not agree. Allow them to elaborate and prove their point.
- Try to understand the position and frame of reference of those holding contrary views.
- Consider how they feel, their concerns and aspirations. Never trivialise their emotions.
- Focus on the substance of the contrary information and the situations leading to the disagreement. Avoid focusing on the motivations of those involved.
- Consider in what ways a strongly held belief might be wrong. Consider the consequences that might occur if it is and if the contrary view is right.
- When there is disagreement, consider whether it is a trade-off situation, a balancing act or a case of different but complementary approaches that can be integrated to achieve common goals.
How views get polarised
CONFIRMATORY bias becomes troubling when policymakers and citizens accumulate facts selectively, making them highly resistant to alternative views.
This often happens to likeminded and close-knit members in a group. The group may be an online community commenting on social and political issues or an advocacy group pursuing a common cause. It could also be a team of civil servants formulating a policy, or a political party discussing strategic policy directions.
Group members reinforce each other’s views, and so the group is quick to reject contrary information. It reaches premature conclusions and tends to hold extreme positions that members would not have held as individuals.
The consequence is that an initially mild disagreement between policymakers and citizens or advocacy groups can easily lead to a polarisation of attitudes.
Confirmatory bias partly explains why some citizens find conspiracy theories or extremely negative interpretations of a government’s intent and motivation believable regardless of the objective evidence.
It also partly explains why some policymakers do not understand citizen sentiments, despite soliciting and receiving feedback from the ground, or having access to independent information that contradicts the policy position.
But it is possible to treat new information in ways that enable disagreements to shed light and not just create heat.
When debating issues such as Internet regulation or a proposed minimum wage, each side should react constructively to contrary information.
Creating a straw man by misrepresenting an opponent’s position or argument – either intentionally or out of ignorance – is counter productive.
When all sides react constructively to contrary information, the Government demonstrates the sort of principled adaptive leadership that is expected of national leaders, and the people display the sort of responsible behaviour and commitment to Singapore that is expected of the citizenry.
There is a need for both principled adaptive leaders and committed responsible citizens if the Government and Singaporeans are to be true co-creators of Singapore’s future.
The writer is director of the Behavioural Sciences Institute, Lee Kuan Yew fellow and professor of psychology at the Singapore Management University.
No comments:
Post a Comment